Skip to main content

Malaria illness mediated by anaemia lessens cognitive development in younger Ugandan children

Abstract

Background

Asymptomatic falciparum malaria is associated with poorer cognitive performance in African schoolchildren and intermittent preventive treatment of malaria improves cognitive outcomes. However, the developmental benefits of chemoprevention in early childhood are unknown. Early child development was evaluated as a major outcome in an open-label, randomized, clinical trial of anti-malarial chemoprevention in an area of intense, year-round transmission in Uganda.

Methods

Infants were randomized to one of four treatment arms: no chemoprevention, daily trimethoprimā€“sulfamethoxazole, monthly sulfadoxineā€“pyrimethamine, or monthly dihydroartemisininā€“piperaquine (DP), to be given between enrollment (4ā€“6Ā mos) and 24Ā months of age. Number of malaria episodes, anaemia (HbĀ <Ā 10) and neurodevelopment [Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL)] were assessed at 2Ā years (NĀ =Ā 469) and at 3Ā years of age (NĀ =Ā 453); at enrollment 70Ā % were HIV-unexposed uninfected (HUU) and 30Ā % were HIV-exposed uninfected (HEU).

Results

DP was highly protective against malaria and anaemia, although trial arm was not associated with MSEL outcomes. Across all treatment arms, episodes of malarial illness were negatively predictive of MSEL cognitive performance both at 2 and 3Ā years of age (PĀ =Ā 0.02). This relationship was mediated by episodes of anaemia. This regression model was stronger for the HEU than for the HUU cohort. Compared to HUU, HEU was significantly poorer on MSEL receptive language development irrespective of malaria and anaemia (PĀ =Ā 0.01).

Conclusions

Malaria with anaemia and HIV exposure are significant risk factors for poor early childhood neurodevelopment in malaria-endemic areas in rural Africa. Because of this, comprehensive and cost/effective intervention is needed for malaria prevention in very young children in these settings.

Background

The adverse effects of both cerebral malaria and severe malaria anaemia in preschool-age children has also been documented [1, 2]. However, uncomplicated malaria has also been associated with cognitive deficits in children. Vitor-Silva et al. concluded that uncomplicated malaria was associated with poorer school performance in the Brazilian Amazon [3]. In Sri Lanka in a series of studies by Fernando and colleagues, poorer academic performance and lower cognitive ability were associated with a higher number of malaria attacks in school children [4ā€“7].

Intermittent prevention treatment (IPT) for malaria in schools and in communities has had cognitive and academic benefits in rural Gambia [8], western Kenya [9], Mali [10], and Zambia [11, 12], although one negative outcome was reported in a Kenyan study [13]. All these IPT clinical trials involved school-age children randomized by classrooms, schools or villages. The developmental benefits of IPT, randomized to individual children in early childhood, are unknown.

The principal study questions are whether: (1) malaria illness and anaemia are related to poorer cognitive development in preschool-age children in an area of high malaria transmission; and, (2) whether perinatally HIV-exposed uninfected (HEU) children more vulnerable to the adverse effects of malarial illness on cognitive development, compared to HIV-unexposed uninfected (HUU) children. FigureĀ 1 depicts the principal factors in the present study analyses (SES/Caregiving, Nutrition/Micronutrient Deficiencies and Anemia, Infectious Disease/Malaria/HIV) influencing brain development, which is measured by the MSEL composite score for the cognitive scales (visual reception, fine motor, receptive language, and expressive language).

Fig.Ā 1
figure 1

Brain development model as evaluated in the present study. The developing brain is measured by the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) composite of the cognitive scales (visual reception, fine motor, receptive language, and expressive language). The measure of socio-economic status (SES) includes quality of caregiving (nurture/neglect) and physical growth (weight-for-age and height-for-age z scores) along with anaemia are partly the result of nutrition and micronutrients. Anaemia is also partly caused by the effects of infectious disease in early development, such as malaria and exposure to maternal HIV

Methods

Study site

This observational study of neurodevelopmental outcomes was embedded within a malaria chemoprevention randomized controlled trial (RCT) study conducted at the Tororo District Hospital near the Kenyan border in eastern Uganda [14, 15] (ClinicalTrials.gov number NCT00948896). Ethical approval was obtained from the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology, the Makerere University School of Medicine Research and Ethics Committee, and the Biomedical Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Michigan State University. Written informed consent was obtained from the principal caregiver (usually the biological mother) for each study child.

Clinical trial of chemoprevention

All children still enrolled at 2Ā years of age in the RCT trials were eligible to be co-enrolled in the present study for MSEL assessment to take place at 2 and 3Ā years of age, except for those who had previously been hospitalized for severe malaria during the RCT study. For the RCT study, eligibility criteria for the HEU children included confirmed HIV-positive status of biological mother; confirmed negative HIV DNA PCR test of infant at time of enrolment; infant actively breastfeeding at the time of enrolment; residency within 30Ā km of the study clinic with no intention of moving outside the study area; agreement to come to the study clinic for any illness and to avoid medications outside the study protocol; provision of informed consent by parent/guardian; no history of allergy or sensitivity to any study drugs; absence of active medical problem requiring in-patient evaluation or chronic medical conditions requiring frequent attention; and absence of clinically significant electrocardiogram abnormalities, family history of long QT syndrome or current use of drugs that prolong the QT interval [15]. Only one eligible child was enrolled per household.

The HEU cohort mothers were on highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) during pregnancy and their study children were on nevirapine for 6Ā weeks and trimethoprimā€“sulfamethoxazole (TS) prophylaxis for the duration of breastfeeding, which is the standard of care in many African countries, including Uganda [16]. These HEU children were enrolled at about four to 5Ā months of age (medianĀ =Ā 4.7Ā months) and monitored on a monthly basis. Six weeks following cessation of breastfeeding, a DNA PCR test was done for each participant and those who remained HIV uninfected were randomized to one of four regimens: no chemoprevention, daily TS, monthly sulfadoxineā€“pyrimethamine (SP), or monthly dihydroartemisininā€“piperaquine (DP). For the HEU children cessation of breast feeding and subsequent randomization to RCT treatment arm ranged from six to 19Ā months of age (medianĀ =Ā 9.7Ā months).

Eligibility criteria for the HUU children in the original RCT trial included the following [14]: (1) born to HIV-uninfected mothers, (2) residency within 30Ā km of the study clinic with no intention of moving outside the study area, (3) agreement to come to the study clinic for any illness and to avoid medications outside the study protocol, (4) provision of informed consent by parent/guardian, (5) no history of allergy or sensitivity to any study drugs, (6) absence of active medical problem requiring in-patient evaluation or chronic medical conditions requiring frequent attention, and (7) absence of clinically significant electrocardiogram (ECG) abnormalities, family history of long QT syndrome, and current use of drugs that prolong the QTc interval. Only one eligible child was enrolled per house.

The HUU study children in the original RCT trial were enrolled at 4ā€“5Ā months of age (medianĀ =Ā 5.6Ā months) and randomized to treatment arms at 6Ā months of age. Both the HEU and HUU children were followed for all their health care needs including monthly routine visits. Chemoprevention was discontinued at 2Ā years of age and children were followed for one additional year until they reached 3Ā years of age. The primary outcomes were the incidence of malaria and incidence of moderate-severe anaemia (haemoglobin <80Ā gm/dL).

At enrolment in the original RCT trial, caregivers were given two long-lasting, insecticide-treated bed nets (LLINs). Anthelminthic, iron sulfate and vitamin A were prescribed following Integrated Management of Childhood Illnesses guidelines. During follow-up, children who presented with a documented fever (tympanic temperature >38Ā Ā°C) or a history of fever in the previous 24Ā h, had blood obtained by finger prick for a thick blood smear. Episodes of uncomplicated malaria were treated with standard doses of artemetherā€“lumefantrine (AL). Episodes of complicated malaria or treatment failures occurring within 14Ā days of therapy were treated with quinine. Routine evaluations, including thick blood smears and assessment of adherence to LLINs and study drugs, were done monthly [15].

Study procedure

FiguresĀ 2 and 3 are CONSORT diagrams depicting the manner in which 186 HEU and 393 HUU children were randomized into 4 malaria chemoprevention treatment arms. The published study findings for the HUU [14] and HEU [15] cohorts contain the detailed methodology for the malaria chemoprevention RCT within which the present neurodevelopment observational study was embedded. ITNs were distributed to all study participants at the time of enrollment. At the time of each monthly routine visit, parents/guardians were asked whether their child slept under an ITN the prior evening. At the time of treatment allocation, parents/guardians were given a 2Ā month supply of study drugs and a diary with dates for dosing and check-offs to indicate administration. Study drugs were resupplied to maintain a 2Ā month supply during clinic visits. At the time of each monthly routine visit, parents/guardians were asked how many doses of study drugs were given since the last routine visit and dates of administration were recorded.

Fig.Ā 2
figure 2

This is the CONSORT diagram showing the process whereby 186 perinatally HIV-exposed and uninfected children (HEU) at 4ā€“5Ā months of age were enrolled in a randomized controlled trial (RCT) to evaluate the effects of four different malaria chemoprevention treatment arms on incidence of malaria illness and levels of anaemia. At 2Ā years of age, these children malaria chemoprevention was discontinued and children co-enrolled in the present study (NĀ =Ā 143) were evaluated with the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). All RCT children continued to be monitored for malaria illness and anaemia until 3Ā years of age, at which time remaining eligible children were again evaluated with the MSEL (NĀ =Ā 122)

Fig.Ā 3
figure 3

This is the CONSORT diagram showing the process whereby 393 perinatally HIV-unexposed and uninfected children (HUU) at 5ā€“6Ā months of age were enrolled in a randomized controlled trial (RCT) to evaluate the effects of four different malaria chemoprevention treatment arms on incidence of malaria illness and levels of anaemia. At 2Ā years of age, these children malaria chemoprevention was discontinued and children co-enrolled in the present study (NĀ =Ā 325) were evaluated with the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). All RCT children continued to be monitored for malaria illness and anaemia until 3Ā years of age, at which time remaining eligible children were again evaluated with the MSEL along with those co-enrolled between 2 and 3Ā years who were missed at the earlier 2-year of age assessment (NĀ =Ā 331)

At 2Ā years of age, all HEU and HUU children not lost to follow-up in the RCT trials, which had not been hospitalized with a history of severe malaria and with caregivers willing to participate, were co-enrolled into our observational study for neurodevelopmental assessment with the MSEL. Children who had been hospitalized with severe malaria were excluded from the present study because of previous findings documenting the relationship between severe malaria and MSEL performance [1, 17]. As can be seen in Fig.Ā 2, 143 out of 186 HEU children were co-enrolled from the malaria chemoprevention RCT for MSEL testing at 2Ā years of age. In Fig.Ā 3, 325 out of 393 HUU RCT children were co-enrolled for MSEL testing at 2Ā years of age. These combined totals for the RCT cohort samples ensured 80Ā % power for our MSEL neurodevelopmental outcomes based on prior work in Tororo [18].

Childrenā€™s neurodevelopment was assessed with the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) twice; at 2 and again at 3Ā years of age. This assessment is intended for use in assessing children from birth to 68Ā months and provides assessment scores for the neurodevelopmental domains of gross motor (up to 33Ā months), and a composite score for the cognitive scales of visual reception, fine motor, receptive language, and expressive language. This test was designed to be more easily adaptable to resource-limited and cross-cultural settings than the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, considered to be the gold standard of such assessments [19]. At the same time, the MSEL has excellent correspondence validity with the Bayley scales [20]. The MSEL has been previously administered in local languages to evaluate the neurodevelopmental benefits of a caregiver training intervention with mothers of HEU children in Tororo [18]. The MSEL has also proven sensitive to the neurodevelopmental effects of severe malaria anaemia and cerebral malaria in Ugandan children [1, 17].

Research assistants administering the MSEL in the present study were all psychology Bachelorā€™s degree graduates of Makerere University fluent in the local languages who underwent 1Ā week of training on the test followed by a month of daily practice testing observation and participation. Following this training and practice period, the head of the assessment team at the study site observed the MSEL administration by the apprentice research assistant and approved the assistant for the assessment of study children contingent on good testing skills and satisfactory completion of an evaluation of MSEL testing skills as observed thereafter on a monthly basis (quality assurance program).

Socio-economic status (SES) was evaluated in the present study using a questionnaire read to the mother in the local language which asked a series of questions about the physical quality of the dwelling (materials and construction of floor, roof, walls, doors, windows, cooking facilities, toilet facilities), availability of water and plumbing, availability of electricity and sources of fuel for cooking/heating, material possessions such as common appliances, radio/TV, bicycle, dishes, furniture, and books. The mother was also asked a series of questions about the types of employment for pay for her and the father/husband or other adult household members, as well as domestic animals available to the household. The SES questionnaire asked about food security in the way of sources of meat, fish, poultry, agriculture and garden produce. Finally, the SES questionnaire for the present study included a set of questions about daily caregiving practices in the home for the study child (e.g., do you play with the child or read to the child most days? Does the child have toys to play with in the home? Are you with the child most of the day and who is with the child when you are not there?).

Measures from the SES questionnaire (Caregiving, Food Security and Nutrition) along with clinic-based measures of anemia and malaria illness were included along with maternal HIV status in a conceptual model of child neurodevelopment depicted in Fig.Ā 1. This conceptual model for the present study guided the statistical analyses described below for the principal study measures and MSEL outcomes.

Statistical analyses

Although in the published findings of the RCT study, the principal outcome was malaria illness incidence per person year at risk (PYAR) [14, 15], counts of malaria episodes were used as the primary exposure variable in the present analyses. This is because the PYAR measure was possibly compromised due to the aggressive treatment course in response to all bouts of malaria, as reported in the RCT study. This included the inclusion of a mandatory 6-week period of parasite clearance verification for each treatment episode for each child. However, substituting the PYAR measure for simple number of malaria episodes did not change any of the principal findings from the analyses described below.

The principal predictor in the present analyses was incidence of malaria, defined as the number of episodes during the period the chemoprevention was given, and between 2 and 3Ā years of age after chemoprevention was stopped. The other exposure variable was incidence of anaemia during the study period: the number of incidents of moderate-severe anaemia (haemoglobin <10Ā gm/dL) during monthly clinic visits during the chemoprevention period (up to 2Ā years) and during the year following cessation of chemoprevention (between 2 and 3Ā years of age).

Primary analyses for the first research question about the relation of malaria illness and anaemia with cognitive development was addressed using linear mixed effects (LME) models. The numbers of malaria and anaemia episodes before 2Ā years, and between 2 and 3Ā years of age were used as time varying covariates. Other time-varying covariates included age, weight for age z-score (WAZ) and number of breastfeeding days in a time period. Fixed (not time-varying) covariates included trial arm, HIV exposure, sex, socio-economic status (SES), and number of observation days prior to randomization. Height-for-age z scores (HAZ) was also added to the regression models (TablesĀ 2, 3, 4, 5) but was not included in the final analyses because of collinearity issues with WAZ. Substituting HAZ for WAZ did not change any of our principal findings with respect to the relationship between malaria and MSEL outcomes with or without anemia.

The assumption of linearity of the relationship between the Mullen scores and principal exposure variables (number of malaria and anemia episodes) was evaluated using scatterplots. To assess the robustness of findings, sensitivity analyses were performed using several cut-offs in the number of episodes. The results obtained from models with categorical exposure variables were not appreciably different from the results from the models with linear effects.

To assess the effect of exposures and the extent to which the effects of the number of malaria episodes were mediated by the anaemia, three LME models were fit.

Model 1 related the MSEL scores (one at a time) to time period (enrolment to 2Ā years, or 2ā€“3Ā years), number of malaria episodes and other covariates described above. The number of anaemia episodes was not included in this model to assess the main effect of the malaria episodes.

Model 2 related the MSEL scores (one at a time) to time period (enrolment to 2Ā years, or 2ā€“3Ā years), number of malaria episodes and other covariates. The number of malaria episodes was not included in this model to assess the main effect of the anaemia episodes.

Model 3 related the MSEL scores (one at a time) to time period (enrolment to 2Ā years, or 2ā€“3Ā years), number of malaria episodes, number of anaemia episodes, and other covariates. This model assessed the effects of malaria and anaemia episodes over and above each other. To establish mediation, the effect of the number of malaria episodes had to be significant in Model 1. If in Model 3 the effect of the number of malaria episodes was no longer significant, then complete mediation was deemed to occur [21ā€“25]. If the magnitude of the effect of the number of malaria episodes was diminished, but was still significant after the inclusion of anaemia, then partial mediation had occurred. To gauge any effects that may differ according to the HIV status, Models 1ā€“3 were run again separately for the HEU and HUU children. All analyses were performed in SAS 9.4.

Results

The subgroup of children from the RCT cohorts (HUU or HEU) who were not co-enrolled in the neurodevelopmental observational MSEL study did not differ statistically from the present study samples on any of the descriptive measures in TableĀ 1. In this table, the HEU children did more poorly than the HUU children on the MSEL receptive language and the MSEL composite (cognitive development) measure at 3Ā years of age, but not 2Ā years (PĀ <Ā 0.01). HEU children had fewer bouts of malaria during both the chemoprevention phase (6Ā months to 2Ā years) (tĀ =Ā 5.94, PĀ <Ā 0.001) and post prevention (2ā€“3Ā years) (tĀ =Ā 3.78, PĀ <Ā 0.001). The HEU children also had fewer bouts of anaemia before 2Ā years of age (tĀ =Ā 2.35, PĀ =Ā 0.02), and fewer exclusively breastfeeding days both before and during the study (tĀ =Ā 7.47, PĀ <Ā 0.001) (see TableĀ 1). The HEU children did more poorly than the HUU cohort on the MSEL at 3Ā years of age (TableĀ 1). They were significantly lower on the MSEL composite cognitive total (tĀ =Ā 3.19, PĀ =Ā 0.002), receptive language (tĀ =Ā 3.07, PĀ <Ā 002), and visual reception (tĀ =Ā 1.97, PĀ <Ā 0.05). The HEU cohort also did more poorly than the HUU children on the MSEL expressive language scale at 2Ā years of age (tĀ =Ā 1.98, PĀ <Ā 0.05) (TableĀ 1).

TableĀ 1 Descriptive statistics for the study sample for perinatally HIV-unexposed and uninfected (HUU) and exposed and uninfected (HEU)

TableĀ 2 presents the mean and standard deviation for the number of malaria and anaemia episodes during the year following chemoprevention, and also includes the MSEL average group performance by treatment arm at 2 and 3Ā years of age. As was the case in the principal publication of RCT results for both cohorts separately [14, 15], DP and TS both resulted in fewer incidences of malaria during chemoprevention treatment compared to SP and the no treatment arm (FĀ =Ā 12.47, PĀ <Ā 0.001 for HUU; FĀ =Ā 9.83, PĀ <Ā 0.001 for HEU). These treatment-arm differences in malaria episodes no longer occurred after treatment was stopped at 2Ā years of age (TableĀ 2). Episodes of anaemia (haemoglobin <10Ā gm/dL) were also fewer for the DP arm during the chemoprevention phase, but only for the HUU cohort (FĀ =Ā 4.90, PĀ =Ā 0.002). After cessation of treatment, there were no longer significant differences among treatment arms on episodes of anaemia. There were no significant differences among treatment arms on the MSEL composite total either following chemoprevention (2Ā years of age assessment) or a year after chemoprevention (at 3Ā years of age) (TableĀ 2).

TableĀ 2 Malaria and anaemia episodes and Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) scores by chemoprevention trial arm

In TableĀ 3 (HUU and HEU combined), malaria episodes were related to MSEL scores at 2 and 3Ā years of age when not controlling for anaemia episodes (Model 1) and when controlling for anaemia episodes (Model 3). This Table also evaluates the extent to which anaemia episodes (during and after chemoprevention) predict MSEL when not controlling for malaria episodes (Model 2) and controlling for malaria episodes (Model 3). Malaria episodes were significantly related to the MSEL composite score (PĀ =Ā 0.02), but not when controlling for anaemia episodes (PĀ =Ā 0.13), suggesting that anaemia mediates this relationship. The same is true for the MSEL outcomes of fine motor (PĀ <Ā 0.01 to PĀ =Ā 0.03) and receptive language (PĀ =Ā 0.04 to PĀ =Ā 0.15). Similar mediational effects were observed when comparing the relationship of anaemia episodes to MSEL with and without controlling for malaria episodes. This was the case for composite cognitive ability (PĀ =Ā 0.01 to PĀ =Ā 0.10), visual reception (PĀ <Ā 0.01 to PĀ =Ā 0.02) and receptive language (PĀ =Ā 0.03 to PĀ =Ā 0.14) when exploring the malaria and anaemia episode effects (TableĀ 3).

TableĀ 3 Malaria and anaemia episodes effects on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) scores for both cohorts combined (HUU and HEU)

Trial arm was not significantly related to the Mullen scores over and above other variables in the models, notably after accounting for the numbers of malaria and/or anaemia episodes. Over and above the malaria and anaemia exposure variables and other covariates, the HUU versus HEU status variable was only significant for the receptive language (favouring the unexposed). Significant predictors included age (negative association), and time (lower scores at 3Ā years of age compared to 2Ā years).

TableĀ 4 presents the effects of the number of malaria and anaemia episodes in statistical Models 1ā€“3 for just the HUU cohort. In this Table, the only MSEL score significantly related to malaria episodes was fine motor, which was mediated by anaemia episodes (PĀ =Ā 0.04 to PĀ =Ā 0.18). A similar mediational effect was observed when evaluating the relationship between anaemia episodes and MSEL fine motor (PĀ =Ā 0.03 to PĀ =Ā 0.14). For TableĀ 5 (HEU cohort), malaria episodes were predictive of MSEL cognitive composite as mediated by anaemia episodes (PĀ =Ā 0.02 to PĀ =Ā 0.10), as well as predictive of receptive language (PĀ =Ā 0.01 to PĀ =Ā 0.05). For this cohort, anaemia episodes only predicted MSEL visual reception as mediated by malaria episodes (PĀ =Ā 0.04 to PĀ =Ā 0.13).

TableĀ 4 Malaria and anaemia episodes effects on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) scores for the HUU cohort
TableĀ 5 Malaria and anaemia episodes effects on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) scores for the HEU cohort

Discussion

Within the context of a rigorous RCT study in a high-malaria-exposure area in rural eastern Uganda, strong evidence is presented that repeated episodes of malaria illness, as mediated by anaemia, can diminish cognitive development in very young children. In the present findings, malaria chemoprevention during early childhood is not directly related to child neurodevelopment. However, malaria episodes during and after chemoprevention are significantly predictive of both cognitive and motor development at 2 and 3Ā years of age. This study is the first to document within the rigorous context of an RCT study that early uncomplicated malaria illness before 2 and 3Ā years of age can diminish cognitive development in very young African children. Furthermore, this relationship is at least partially mediated by anaemia. However, another form of analysis that might have been used to better understand the mediating effects of anemia is structural equation modeling. Future work could employ this statistical method as well.

HIV exposure resulted in lower MSEL scores on composite cognitive performance at 3Ā years of age, especially receptive and expressive language. MSEL differences were observed at 2Ā years of age only at the 0.05 significance level on Expressive Language (PĀ <Ā 0.05). These differences may have been due to differing levels of sensitivity of these scales to the developmental effects of HIV-exposure in this setting, depending on which developmental domains blossom most at a given year of age. For example, MSEL language development differences for HEU children compared to HUU children in rural Uganda has been previously observed in the pre-school age years [26].

It should also be noted that the relationship between episodes of malaria illness and MSEL composite cognitive and receptive language performance for the HEU cohort was more robust than for the HUU cohort in the present study (comparing TablesĀ 4, 5), when the MSEL outcomes were considered at 3Ā years of age. Cognitive development risk from malaria and anaemia seems to be greater in HEU children, especially as they enter into the critical preschool-age years at 3Ā years of age. However, the clinical significance of the statistically significant relationships observed in the present study between malaria illness and MSEL developmental outcomes is uncertain. This is because true Uganda-based norms are missing for the MSEL, and only American-based norms are available for standardizing performance on the basis of age and gender.

Despite this more robust relationship, the overall number of episodes of malaria illness and anaemia during and after chemoprevention was lower for the HEU than for the HUU cohorts (TableĀ 1). This is likely due to the fact that in the RCT study within which this observational study was embedded, the HEU children were maintained on TS while nursing, rather than being weaned and randomized to the chemoprevention treatment arms at 6Ā months of age as were the HUU children. Both TS and DP chemoprevention significantly reduced the number of malaria episodes compared to the SP and placebo treatment arms [14, 15, 27].

Although a significant relationship was observed between episodes of malaria illness and neurodevelopmental outcomes in the present study findings, significant neurodevelopmental differences were not observed among chemoprevention treatment arms, either during or after the treatment phase. This might be partly due to questionable adherence to the treatment arm regimens on the part of the mothers of the children, as witnessed by the results of monitoring of DP blood levels in the RCT trial [14, 15]. Chemoprevention treatment adherence was mostly monitored by self-report on the part of the mothers during their monthly clinic visits. Such adherence issues would also pertain to the use of prophylactic antibiotics and micronutrient supplements provided for participants during the study period.

Finally, socio-economic and educational status can impact on both the reliable use of malaria chemoprevention medications and quality of housing in terms of night-time exposure of study children to mosquitoes [28]. In the previous study of HEU children in Tororo, SES, maternal educational level, nutritional quality, housing quality, and maternal depression were all related to child neurodevelopment independent of malaria episodes per se [18]. Duration of breast feeding is related to morbidity for both the HEU and HUU children in the study cohorts [29]. These important factors related to child development may not have been equally distributed across chemoprevention treatment arms in this observational study within the larger RCT study.

The present study findings support other studies with school-age children that conclude that repeated episodes of malaria are associated with poor cognitive scores and low academic performance [9, 30]. Even asymptomatic positive malaria parasitaemia led to poorer cognitive performance among school-age children at the Tororo study site [31]. Unfortunately, the relationship between asymptomatic positive parasitaemia and MSEL measures of neurodevelopment could not be evaluated in the present study. Whenever children developed a fever and had a positive blood smear the day before or at their monthly clinic visit, they were treated with AL and monitored for parasite clearance following treatment.

Because convenience sampling was used to enroll the original HEU cohort of 200 and the HUU cohort of 400 infants at 4ā€“5 mo of age from the Tororo District Hospital Maternal and Child Health Clinic their birth records were incomplete. Therefore, premature and low-birth weight children could not be excluded and this may have led to developmental delays that could compromise the sensitivity of the MSEL outcomes to the effects of malaria illness frequency and anaemia in the present samples. HEU children in this study area are at significantly greater risk for premature birth and complications, and shorter duration of breast feeding [32]. These risk factors can lead to developmental delays at measured by the MSEL [26]. Maternal depression for mothers with HIV can also affect MSEL outcomes for HEU children, and this was not measured for mothers in the present study [33]. This could also obscure the relationship between malaria or anaemia and MSEL outcomes in the present analyses and serve as a study limitation.

Beyond LLIN distribution programmes, little has been done for very young children in Africa to protect them against malaria. In rural households in Tororo District which have been provided with LLINs, children can still have as high as 310 infectious bites per year with a malaria-positive parasite prevalence rate of 28.7Ā % [28]. In the present study cohorts, where LLINs and prompt effective treatment were provided, the risk of complicated malaria and anaemia was extremely low. However, incidence of malaria illness was still high with the present standard of care (SP) (malaria illnessesĀ >Ā seven times/year) and increased as soon as more effective chemoprevention (DP and TS) was discontinued. This suggests that more is needed in early malaria prevention than simply improved LLIN distribution. This includes preventing malaria exposure for mothers during pregnancy, especially mothers with HIV [34].

In the longer term, malaria vaccines may have an important role in protecting this important section of the community from malaria. Regardless of the control approach selected, it is important that a comprehensive and aggressive community-wide approach to controlling malaria be incorporated into every public health programme in malaria-endemic regions, as part of the ongoing effort to enhance the health of African school-age children [30].

Conclusions

Early bouts of malaria illness can diminish cognitive development in very young children. These effects of malaria illness on cognition are partly mediated by anaemia, and cognitive development risk from malaria and anaemia seems to be greater in HEU children. This is the case even with children and caregivers who are provided with free and effective medical care and nutritional support in the first years of life. The high level of malaria transmission and the host of other risk factors in such low-resource environments can still overwhelm such medical and nutritional support, even when provided in a conscientious manner in such settings.

Availability of supporting data

We will be able to download our de-identified datasets and data definition dictionary from our university data management archive to make them available to other research groups upon request. We will require a collaborative agreement from such groups before making such datasets available, as approved through the Intellectual Property Office at Michigan State University. See http://newsroom.msu.edu/site/indexer/3004/content.htm for an overview of this office and http://oip.msu.edu/documents/CDA2001_002.pdf for an example of such an agreement. In addition, we will ask for final approval of all publications from other research groups that include our study data.

Abbreviations

AL:

artemetherā€“lumefantrine

ANCOVA:

analysis of covariance

DP:

dihydroartemisininā€“piperaquine

HAART:

highly active anti-retroviral therapy

HAZ:

height-for-age z score

HIV:

human immunodeficiency virus

IPT:

intermittent preventive treatment

IRB:

Institutional Review Board

LLIN:

long-lasting insecticide-treated bed net

LME:

linear mixed-effects model

MSEL:

Mullen Scales of Early Learning

HEU:

perinatally HIV-exposed and uninfected

HUU:

perinatally HIV-unexposed and uninfected

PYAR:

person-year at risk

SES:

socio-economic status

SP:

sulfadoxineā€“pyrimethamine

TS:

trimethoprimā€“sulfamethoxazole

UNCST:

Ugandan National Council for Science and Technology

WAZ:

weight-for-age adjusted Z score

WHO:

World Health Organization

References

  1. Bangirana P, Opoka RO, Boivin MJ, Idro R, Hodges JS, Romero RA, et al. Severe malarial anemia is associated with longterm neurocognitive impairment. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59:336ā€“44.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  2. Boivin MJ, Gladstone MJ, Maclean V, Birbeck GL, Magen JG, Page C, et al. Developmental outcomes in Malawian children with retinopathy-positive cerebral malaria. Trop Med Int Health. 2011;16:263ā€“71.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Vitor-Silva S, Reyes-Lecca RC, Pinheiro TR, Lacerda MV. Malaria is associated with poor school performance in an endemic area of the Brazilian Amazon. Malar J. 2009;8:230.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  4. Fernando D, de Silva D, Carter R, Mendis KN, Wickremasinghe R. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, clinical trial of the impact of malaria prevention on the educational attainment of school children. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2006;74:386ā€“93.

    PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  5. Fernando D, Wickremasinghe R, Mendis KN, Wickremasinghe AR. Cognitive performance at school entry of children living in malaria-endemic areas of Sri Lanka. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg. 2003;97:161ā€“5.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Fernando SD, Gunawardena DM, Bandara MR, De Silva D, Carter R, Mendis KN, et al. The impact of repeated malaria attacks on the school performance of children. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2003;69:582ā€“8.

    CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  7. Fernando SD, Rodrigo C, Rajapakse S. The ā€˜hiddenā€™ burden of malaria: cognitive impairment following infection. Malar J. 2010;9:366.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  8. Jukes MC, Pinder M, Grigorenko EL, Smith HB, Walraven G, Bariau EM, et al. Long-term impact of malaria chemoprophylaxis on cognitive abilities and educational attainment: follow-up of a controlled trial. PLoS Clin Trials. 2006;1:e19.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  9. Clarke SE, Jukes MC, Njagi JK, Khasakhala L, Cundill B, Otido J, et al. Effect of intermittent preventive treatment of malaria on health and education in schoolchildren: a cluster-randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet. 2008;372:127ā€“38.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  10. Thuilliez J, Sissoko MS, Toure OB, Kamate P, Berthelemy JC, Doumbo OK. Malaria and primary education in Mali: a longitudinal study in the village of Doneguebougou. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71:324ā€“34.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  11. Fink G, Dickens WT, Jordan M, Cohen JL. Access to subsidized ACT and malaria treatment-evidence from the first year of the AMFm program in six districts in Uganda. Health Policy Plan. 2014;29:517ā€“27.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  12. Fink G, Olgiati A, Hawela M, Miller JM, Matafwali B. Association between early childhood exposure to malaria and childrenā€™s pre-school development: evidence from the Zambia early childhood development project. Malar J. 2013;12:12.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  13. Halliday KE, Okello G, Turner EL, Njagi K, McHaro C, Kengo J, et al. Impact of intermittent screening and treatment for malaria among school children in Kenya: a cluster randomised trial. PLoS Med. 2014;11:e1001594.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Bigira V, Kapisi J, Clark TD, Kinara S, Mwangwa F, Muhindo MK, et al. Protective efficacy and safety of three antimalarial regimens for the prevention of malaria in young Ugandan children: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS Med. 2014;11:e1001689.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  15. Kamya MR, Kapisi J, Bigira V, Clark TD, Kinara S, Mwangwa F, et al. Efficacy and safety of three regimens for the prevention of malaria in young HIV-exposed Ugandan children: a randomized controlled trial. AIDS. 2014;28:2701ā€“9.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Church K, Kiweewa F, Dasgupta A, Mwangome M, Mpandaguta E, GĆ³mez-OlivĆ© FX, et al. A comparative analysis of national HIV policies in six African countries with generalized epidemics. Bull World Health Organ. 2015;93:457ā€“67.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  17. Bangirana P, Opoka RO, Boivin MJ, Idro R, Hodges JS, John CC. Neurocognitive domains affected by cerebral malaria and severe malarial anemia in children. Learn Individ Differ. 2015. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.01.010.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  18. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Nakasujja N, Page CF, Shohet C, Givon D, et al. A year-long caregiver training program to improve neurocognition in preschool Ugandan HIV-exposed children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2013;34:269ā€“78.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  19. Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning: AGS edition. Minneapolis, MN: American Guidance Services; 1995.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Kammerer B, Isquith PJ, Lundy S. Approaches to assessment of very young children in Africa in the context of HIV. In: Boivin MJ, Giordani B, editors. Neuropsychology of children in Africa: perspectives on risk and resilience. Specialty topics in neuropsychology. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 17ā€“36.

    ChapterĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderatorā€“mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173ā€“82.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  22. Judd CM, Kenny DA. Process analysis: estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Eval Rev. 1981;5:602ā€“19.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. MacKinnon DP. Analysis of mediating variables in prevention and intervention research. In: Cazares A, Beatty LA, editors. Scientific methods in prevention research. Washington, DC: Government Print Office; 1994. p. 127ā€“53.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  24. MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding, and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000;1:173ā€“81.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  25. MacKinnon DP, Warsi GD, Dwyer JH. A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivar Behav Res. 1995;30:41ā€“62.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  26. Brahmbhatt H, Boivin M, Ssempijja V, Kigozi G, Kagaayi J, Serwadda D, et al. Neurodevelopmental benefits of antiretroviral therapy in Ugandan children aged 0ā€“6Ā years with HIV. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;67:316ā€“22.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Kakuru A, Jagannathan P, Arinaitwe E, Wanzira H, Muhindo M, Bigira V, et al. The effects of ACT treatment and TS prophylaxis on Plasmodium falciparum gametocytemia in a cohort of young Ugandan children. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2013;88:736ā€“43.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  28. Kamya MR, Arinaitwe E, Wanzira H, Katureebe A, Barusya C, Kigozi SP, et al. Malaria transmission, infection, and disease at three sites with varied transmission intensity in Uganda: implications for malaria control. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2015;92:903ā€“12.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Marquez C, Okiring J, Chamie G, Ruel TD, Achan J, Kakuru A, et al. Increased morbidity in early childhood among HIV-exposed uninfected children in Uganda is associated with breastfeeding duration. J Trop Pediatr. 2014;60:434ā€“41.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  30. Nankabirwa J, Brooker SJ, Clarke SE, Fernando D, Gitonga CW, Schellenberg D, et al. Malaria in school-age children in Africa: an increasingly important challenge. Trop Med Int Health. 2014;19:1294ā€“309.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  31. Nankabirwa J, Wandera B, Kiwanuka N, Staedke SG, Kamya MR, Brooker SJ. Asymptomatic plasmodium infection and cognition among primary schoolchildren in a high malaria transmission setting in Uganda. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2013;88:1102ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  32. Vora N, Homsy J, Kakuru A, Arinaitwe E, Wanzira H, Sandison TG, et al. Breastfeeding and the risk of malaria in children born to HIV-infected and uninfected mothers in rural Uganda. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2010;55:253ā€“61.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  33. Bass JK, Nakasujja N, Familiar-Lopez I, Sikorskii A, Murray SM, Opoka R, et al. Association of caregiver quality of care with neurocognitive outcomes in HIV-affected children aged 2ā€“5Ā years in Uganda. AIDS Care. 2016. doi:10.1080/09540121.2016.1146215:1-8 (epub 20-January-2016).

    Google ScholarĀ 

  34. Brahmbhatt H, Sullivan D, Kigozi G, Askin F, Wabwire-Mangenm F, Serwadda D, et al. Association of HIV and malaria with mother-to-child transmission, birth outcomes, and child mortality. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2008;47:472ā€“6.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Authorsā€™ contributions

MJB participated in the design of the study, had oversight of developmental assessment and participated in the analysis of the data, drafted and served as corresponding author for the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. AS was responsible for the statistical analysis for the study, provided study tables, participated in the writing of the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. IF-L supported the developmental assessment project team, data management efforts, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. HR-E supported the developmental assessment project team and approved the final manuscript as submitted. MM, JK and VB provided oversight of the malaria chemoprevention randomized controlled clinical trial, helped supervise the medical care of the children, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. ROO participated in the design of the study, supervising implementation of protocol, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. NN participated in the design of the study, Uganda-based human subjectā€™s protection approval application oversight, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JKB participated in the design of the study and approved the final manuscript as submitted. MK participated in the design of the study, in the design of the IPC randomized clinical trial, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. GD participated in the design of the study, design and oversight of the chemoprevention randomized clinical trial, analysis and interpretation of the malaria illness and anaemia data, drafting of manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The neurodevelopmental assessment portion of this study was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grants HD070723 (MJB, AS, IF-L, HR-E, NN, JKB, ROO). The malaria chemoprevention RCT was supported by HD059454 (VB, JK, MM, MK, GD). Holley-Cotec provided the dihydroartemisininā€“piperaquine for the malaria chemoprevention randomized controlled trial (RCT). The study sponsors had no role in the study design, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, or in the writing of the report and in the decision to submit the paper for publication. Results of this study were previously presented at the 63rd Annual Meeting of the American Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, New Orleans, LA, USA, 3 November, 2014.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J. Boivin.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Boivin, M.J., Sikorskii, A., Familiar-Lopez, I. et al. Malaria illness mediated by anaemia lessens cognitive development in younger Ugandan children. Malar J 15, 210 (2016). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12936-016-1266-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12936-016-1266-x

Keywords